Potential vs. achievement
Robin:
We thought we would talk about potential today.
Agnes:
The preference for potential over actual achievement, right?
Robin:
Exactly. So there's a, I think, 2012 paper. that I blogged in 2015 or
something when I heard about it, that measured this difference across a number
of different contexts, including ads and evaluations of people, and they
varied just the difference between having achieved something and having the
potential to achieve something or estimated to achieve something. And then
across the right range of contacts, they found that people were more
interested in and more eager to meet and interact with people who had
potential to do something than had actually done it, which is surprising, of
course.
Agnes:
And it might not just be about wanting to meet people. It might be broader
than that.
Robin:
A connection with them of some sort, right.
Agnes:
No, it might be broader than that. Okay. If you imagine there's a piece of
technology, like Twitter or something, right? And it has achieved something.
Take ChatGPT, right? It achieves amazing things, but I think a lot of people's
excitement about it is about its potential. And the people that are most
excited about it, they're constantly thinking, yeah, but think about what it's
going to be able to do. They're actually more excited about that than what it
currently can do often. So I think it's not only people, it's broader.
Robin:
Makes sense. So, I mean, we should elaborate why we would be surprised by
this. So the simple idea would be if you were just going to directly use
something, then the fact that it was able to do something would be more of a
reassurance. And the fact that it had a potential would be less reassuring if
you were just looking at the direct. So if you wanted to hire somebody who, I
don't know, could build a new missile or something, If you had somebody who
had already made a missile, that might be more useful than somebody who had
the potential to make a missile. Or if you wanted someone who could win a
Super Bowl, say, somebody who had already won a Super Bowl might sound more
valuable than somebody who had the potential to win a Super Bowl. That would
be the reason why this would be surprising.
Agnes:
Right. So in a case where you know what it is that you want, Then it looks
like what you are excited about is someone or something that can help you
achieve what you want. But if you don't know exactly what you want, and you're
hoping to get something better than what you can currently specify yourself as
wanting, then the potential person is more exciting.
Robin:
So what we're probably going to do in the next few minutes is go through
various possible theories to explain this. And so you've just named one.
Agnes:
I'll just take an example, actually, before we get into theories. The example
that really struck me was when I wrote my first book, which is about
aspiration, the way that I wrote that book was I wrote the book, and then I
sent it to a publisher, and I'm like, here's a book I wrote. Do you want to
publish it? And this is an academic book, and they were like, well, let's send
it to reviewers. They sent it to reviewers. Reviewers liked that they
published it. And then as I was working towards a trade book, I wanted to do
the same thing, but I'm like, well, I found out for a trade book you need an
agent, so I got an agent. And I was like, well, she's like, well, you need to
write a proposal and we need to shop it around. And I said, I have a different
idea. How about I just write the book? And then when I've written it, we can
just ask people, do you want to publish this book? And she's like, no, no, no.
Nobody will ever, ever publish it if you do that. You have to sell them the
idea of a book. You have to sell them a potential book. Nobody wants to buy an
actual book. That is, no publisher wants to buy an actual book. A publisher
wants to buy a potential book, the dream of a book, the idea of a book. That's
what you can sell. And to me, that was that was very surprising. But that's
yeah, that's like a place where this phenomenon has showed up in my life.
Robin:
So let me just give one more example from an interview with somebody who wrote
the paper. They said there's two job applicants that were the same, except
there's a test they were said to have done well on. One of the tests was an
assessment of leadership potential, and the other was an assessment of
leadership achievement. So they're just swapping those two terms and leaving
everything else the same. And then people were more interested in hiring the
person rating the candidate higher if they had scored well on the potential.
Agnes:
Right.
Robin:
All right. So, so one theory that you just introduced is this idea that you
might not know exactly what you want. But that's still somewhat puzzling
because you might think whatever it is you don't want, I mean, this having
achieved something might be more indicative of other positive things. So only
if somehow the potential rating was somehow broader or had a wider potential
than an achievement, I guess.
Agnes:
But it does.
Robin:
It's not obvious. So if you're looking at someone who has potential for, you
know, winning an Olympic medal or something, and somebody has won an Olympic
medal, it's not clear that the potential to win an Olympic medal shows very
many other kinds of abilities that actually winning the medal does.
Agnes:
I mean, that's a bit of a weird case because it's like, do you hire people for
this or whatever? But if you think about the book case, right, the actual book
is a more concrete object than the potential book, which could be any number
of any variety of books. And similarly with leadership, If someone has that
actual leadership, then you know, okay, they could do leadership in these
particular areas, but if they have leadership potential, I mean, they could
lead you anywhere, maybe. It does seem to me that the potential is going to be
vaguer and more open-ended.
Robin:
But it doesn't necessarily indicate a broader potential. It's just more
uncertain, really.
Agnes:
Yes, but more uncertain is broader. Yeah, and you have to actually study your
beliefs and your faith.
Robin:
Okay, let's distinguish two dimensions, right? So there's a bunch of different
ways you could achieve, and then on each axis of achievement, there's how far
you get. And so literally, if you pick one axis or you rate somebody on have
they achieved it or do they have the potential for it, So having the potential
for that particular thing doesn't necessarily mean have more potential for
other things. I would, in fact, guess that if you had achieved A, you would
have more potential for B, C, D, and E than if you only have the potential for
A. That makes sense.
Agnes:
But I think we just actually think about and frame the potential differently
from the actual. So with the actual, we think about a single axis. With the
potential, we don't think about it that way. We think about the many.
Robin:
So that's a sort of cognitive error theory.
Agnes:
I'm not sure it's an error. I mean, it's a difference between the two cases.
Something I think worth rowing in is that, and it's certainly true in the
editorial case, and it's probably true in more cases, is that often with the
potential, you think you can shape it. So the editor, if I give him a book
idea, he gets to have input.
Robin:
So that's a second explanation here.
Agnes:
It would be a reason why thinking of it along many, many axes in a vague way
might not be so wrong.
Robin:
Well, I mean, breadth of ability is just different from potential to shape it.
I think those are just two separate factors. I don't even know if they
correlate, but it is a relevant factor. If you want to shape something, Uh,
that might matter, right? So for example, if you, you know, want to hire a
manager and you want him to become a, uh, you know, acolyte of yours and, and,
uh, somebody who supports you and, and does your sort of style of management,
you might want someone who is shapeable rather than someone who's already been
a good manager while you assume they they're full of themselves and they, they
know what they're doing and they're not going to be very amenable to sort of
becoming like you. So when I was a grad student, I returned to grad school and
was an older grad student. And so I went and visited a professor at Stanford
near where I was to talk about related things. And he went out of his way to
say that most professors are a bit wary of older students because they're
harder to shape. The standard story is that most young students, they're kind
of, you know, not that reliable in the sense they're still very young and they
want to date and they get caught up in hobbies and things like that. And older
students were known for being very serious and getting to work because, you
know, they'd been out of school for a long time and they'd been working and,
you know, this was their big chance and they were definitely going to focus
on, you know, getting some work done. but that they were less pliable. That
is, they would less become a copy of you. And many professors wanted to make a
student who was a copy of them, who would, you know, promote all their work
and do their sorts of things. And older students were just less likely to do
that.
Agnes:
And that's like a cynical framing, but a less cynical framing is like, I've
noticed that when I give a talk, I am more excited about speaking to the
younger students, the younger people in the group than the older people. And
that's because I just cannot influence the old people. Like they decided how
they're gonna interpret these texts, what they meant, what's important, like
20 years ago. And that door is closed in their mind. And I can experience, I
can experience how closed that door is. And so it's like not very exciting for
me to talk to them. The point is not that I want to like impress my view on
the mold of these young minds and have them all become copies of me. They
might, you know, give interesting pushback or whatever as well, but they're
just more like alive to the conversation. Whereas I feel like the old people
hold the conversation at a really far distance from themselves because they've
settled these things long ago.
Robin:
Right. So the authors of this paper we're responding to, and I guess we'll put
a link on the page where we have this podcast, are aware that many people
think that this might be an age effect. And so they go out of their way to
control for age in some of these vignettes to see that it's not just that we
like younger people as more flexible. We also think holding the age constant,
we prefer the person with the potential.
Agnes:
Right. It could be the other way around. I mean, this could probably explain
why we hear about age.
Robin:
Sure. So you've offered two explanations. One is that potential seems broader
and the other is that potential seems more shapeable. So let me offer two
explanations. So first, you might think that potential comes cheaper in two
ways. So if you're hiring this, you know, manager or whatever, you might think
the one who has demonstrated achievement will be more expensive And someone
who just has potential will be cheaper. And so you're trying to sort of get in
cheap, just like you want to buy a stock before it rises. If you're
speculating on investments or something. You don't want to buy a stock when
it's high and everybody agrees it's high. It's, you know, you're not going to
get the gain on its rise. You're, you're hoping to gain on the rise by having
an early association with someone. And then as they rise, you gain from that
rise. So you can gain directly in salary here. I get an employee cheaper, but
you could also just gain in associations. That is, it might be that if you
have an associations with somebody before they become famous. That association
may be stronger or you can lobby them more strongly on something, or they
trust you more or something. Cause there are many stories about celebrities
surrounding themselves with the people they knew before they were became
celebrities. Cause those are the people they trust. So, so that's one
explanation is just you're hoping to get a cheaper price or more, more
benefits out of the same association later by knowing them early. Another
theory is that potential implicitly comes with more social support. So, you
know, I developed this idea of prediction markets, and in part, one of the
scenarios I had always thought about for prediction markets is, you know, what
if somebody is a contrarian and there's a consensus opinion, but they disagree
and they pursue their alternative opinion and later they're proved right. I
noticed that in their current world, they don't tend to get much for later
being proved right. And I thought, well, with prediction markets, they could
get more of a reward for going against the consensus of being proved right.
But I think part of the reason that in our world they don't get rewarded that
much is that we actually, the fact that somebody was contrarian and was proved
right still shows they were a contrarian. and probably indicates that they
don't have as many good connections and associations as the people who were in
the majority agreeing with the majority earlier on. And so also with this
potential case, someone who many people say has the potential to achieve
something that might indicate that, well, they're well connected. They, the
usual authorities say they have potential and that's going to ensure you're
not just about what they'll do, but that they have the support of the usual
authorities and community. Whereas somebody who has achieved something, they
might've achieved that in defiance of the usual authorities, in which case
they might have high ability, but still you might be wary of how other people
treat them and what connections they have and what social support they have
from the usual authorities. So we have four theories on the table now.
Agnes:
Yeah. So I think that actually your first theory about cheaper is related to
my shapeable theory in that like it's both that the potential person is more,
you're able to mold them and influence them and also they're then going to
carry your, that association is going to have this kind of potentially
transformative effect for you. It's like you influence them in the sense that
because you knew them back when, Um, they, you know, esteem you and want to
hold on to you and all of that. Like that's, it's a way of influencing them.
Uh, you're, you're able to connect to them.
Robin:
Right. It's influencing their judgments about who they trust and influencing
their connections, not just their features.
Agnes:
Exactly, that's what I meant to say. Right, so it's another kind of influence
in a way. And it's like, you're not gonna have that kind of influence if you
meet a celebrity for lunch or something, and they're already a celebrity,
you're just another person they're meeting for lunch that they're gonna forget
you right away. If you met them for lunch back when they were struggling, then
you are able to influence their opinion of you and of your importance. So I
think it is about, it's about the shapeability and influence and moldability
in some way of the person
Robin:
and relation.
Agnes:
And the relation of the relation. That's right. And I think that. Yeah, I
mean, so here's like, you know, I've noticed that. It used to be the case,
this isn't really as true anymore because of just how bad the job market has
gotten, but like when I was on the philosophy job market, I was told like,
don't go on the job market until you're real, don't just like, if there's a
couple of jobs you might be interested in, don't just apply for those jobs,
but then wait to go all out the next year because by the next year, everybody
will know that it's your second year and that you were rejected once. and
that's gonna be a mark against you. Similarly, don't apply to an MA program,
because if you go to an MA program and you apply to a PhD program from there,
then it's like, well, you probably applied to PhD programs the first time
around and you were rejected. So there's this kind of sense that the potential
person hasn't yet been rejected. They're fresh meat in that way, and I think
we're very averse to people who have been rejected.
Robin:
So there's a related thing in economics that I heard, which is that, you know,
there are many journals that are, some are ranked very high and some are
ranked middling and some are low. And there's this advice that you should, at
least if you have a shot at some top journal publications, to never apply or
publish elsewhere. That is, if you have, you know, four top publications and
four middling publications, that'll look worse for you than if you just have
four top publications.
Agnes:
Because the middling ones would be rejects from the top ones.
Robin:
Yes, and they say indicate that you are willing to do middling level work.
Agnes:
Right, right, right. So there's a way in which it's just the potential person,
it's like they're in the Garden of Eden and they haven't sinned yet. They're
sort of pure. And you can give them the benefit of the doubt that they would
never apply to those middling journals. You can still have that illusion about
them or something like that. And so somehow the, the evidence of any kind of
failing carries a lot of weight. Even though, of course, an actual successful
person, like any actual successful person is going to have certain kinds of
failings. But we don't have those don't have to be in view yet in the
potential person. We can have the illusion of there being perfect, just like
the potential book that I'm going to write. We can have this illusion of it's
being the perfect book, the exact book my editor most wanted to be written.
You can have that illusion about it. And that might be related to the ways in
which, like, If I, if I write something online and there's a hundred comments
on it and like five of them are negative, those are going to be the ones that
I attend to and that I remember. And so similarly with the actual person,
there's going to be some negativity and we're going to attend to that. Whereas
there might well be just no negativity for the potential person.
Robin:
So this phenomena, I think, helps us understand a bit better the central role
of, say, educational ranking in society more than you might have thought. So,
you know, the story I'd always heard is something like, you know, the people
who get accepted to the best schools or something, they get an in faster at
better places, and then they get more opportunities. And if you're ranked
lower, maybe you won't get a place where you could shine. and you won't get
chances to shine. And so you will lose out because you never really got a
chance. But this suggests that there's a bigger effect than just that, that
even if you do get as much a chance to shine in your later career as the
person who went to the top school, it still might be that people preferred the
person who had the potential early, rather than the person who eventually
achieves the same thing.
Agnes:
Yes, though I think it's complex because there is a bit of a, depending on the
context that you're in, actually, there's a bit of almost like an ethos of,
oh, the person who goes to the state school has their own kind of purity.
Like, they're untainted by the Ivy League. There's definitely gonna be
contexts where that's actually gonna be a plus. I think that's rare.
Robin:
In the world I've seen, that's pretty rare.
Agnes:
Your colleague, Tyler, just did a conversation with an economics educator, and
a big part of the conversation is how she went to the University of Kentucky
or whatever, and the advantages that gave her, instead of going to an Ivy
League school, There's definitely a romanticism about that, and about not
being part of the Ivy League machine. Tyler was channeling that, and was
channeling it for his audience, so there's an audience for that. So I think it
definitely exists. There's just a lot of ... Anytime there's some elite
structure, there's just a lot of backlash to that, and that's just- Right.
Robin:
a tendency to celebrate the people who were contrarians and then who were
proved right anyway, at least verbally, but I still think they seem to lose
overall.
Agnes:
Well, you wouldn't think that. But I think they often win. I mean, I think
they sometimes lose. And it's sort of hard to know, because we don't hear
about a lot of the cases, right? But when people win, OK, when people do win,
they're always insisting on their contrarianness. That is, the winner has
always begun themselves as contrarian. So contrarian is a very popular facade
to put forward. It's a little hard to know. I think you're probably right, if
we took the space of all, if we took all the contrarians, we counted them all,
and we look at how many actually end up winning, it's not gonna be many. But
if you look at the people who succeed, they tend to present themselves as the
beat.
Robin:
I was more conditioning on, you're a contrarian, and then you're proved right,
compared to other people who might be proved right, but they weren't taking a
very contrarian position. They were just proved right about the usual thing
everybody already agreed with.
Agnes:
Yeah, I guess I'm not sure it should count more. I mean, for one thing, you
may have been proved wrong about a lot of other things. I think, in fact, when
people are later proved right, they make a huge deal out of it, but they don't
tell you about all the times they were proved wrong about stuff, so they're
just going to emphasize the ones where they were proved right. So prediction
run gets to be better because you can have a total- Exactly. But, you know,
you might have just taken a whole bunch of contrarian positions instead of
taking a whole bunch of conventional positions. Do you get more credit because
the contrarian ones are right? Like, I don't see why you should.
Robin:
So I guess we're slipping into talking about status, which is something I
wanted to talk about in other episodes anyway, so we could talk about it a
little bit here. But this is an issue of status and prestige in the sense that
we all do love status and prestige and we want it for ourselves and we respect
it and others. But we also like to talk an egalitarian game. And we like to
talk as if we, you know, we're more equal and that we all, everybody was being
listened to and everybody's being included and everybody has a chance. And I
think that's a way in which we're a bit in conflict. Uh, and we actually, I
think do in fact, want to be high status in the usual ways. And in fact,
choose to prefer to associate with people who are higher status, even though
we want to also celebrate egalitarian choices and norms.
Agnes:
So I have, I'm going to make an observation, but then I think we should go
back to our topic because this is a digression, which is I feel like I had an
epiphany over the past few weeks about like the way that I think about it is
two different status clouds that hang over interactions. And so one kind of
status cloud, and I see it much more in humanities contexts, is imposter
syndrome. So people, it's not that they want to be higher status than they
are. It's that they're really scared that they don't deserve the status that
they have. They have anxiety over whether they are really whatever they are.
Do I deserve to be a professor? Do I deserve even to be a graduate student?
Like things where they're not even on high status. Right? First-year graduate
students, it's not like you're somehow super high status, but first-year
graduate students have a lot of this worry, do I deserve to be where I am? Is
my status somehow fake? And then there's, and it's in those contexts that
you'll see a lot of egalitarian talk. Because it's like people are just
reassuring one another, oh, everyone's nervous like you, everyone feels these
fears, you know, of course you deserve to be where you are. So it's one kind
of status cloud, and that involves a lot of reassurance. And then the second
kind that I more often feel that I'm under when I talk to you is people who
think they deserve more status than they have. So you're not anxious about
whether you deserve the status you have, but you think you should have more
than you do. And I think that's more of a thing in tech circles and
non-humanity circles. And that's like, we want status and prestige. We want
opportunities to show off. We're competitive. We want recognition. We think
the world should recognize us more than we do. Whether you're in the one cloud
or the other is not correlated with how much status you have. Because I see
Elon Musk as someone who's deeply in the second cloud.
Robin:
I think it is correlated with a potential tie.
Agnes:
my sentence, that he deserves a lot more status than he has, even though he's
got a ton of status. So, right, so those are the two worlds. Do you deserve
the status that you have, or do you actually deserve more status than you
have?
Robin:
And I don't think that's a digression. I mean, I think the people who are
assigned, you know, the potential rating are the first group you talked about,
and the people who don't get the potential rating but later achieve are the
second group you talked about. And so these are the two groups here that we're
looking at.
Agnes:
The potential people have to the status that they've been. I guess it makes
sense, right? Because I picked first year graduate students.
Robin:
Right. People assigned potential. Well, at least they're the top school.
You're a top school. So somebody at a different school won't be so much rated
for their potential.
Agnes:
Still, you had to get into graduate school and you had to be like, oh, now I'm
a graduate student. Now I'm on the way. Do I deserve to be being on the way to
becoming a professor? So, regardless what school you're at, I think there's
going to be some of that. But yeah, maybe there's more of it at a higher
ranked school. I mean, my school's not that high ranked. We're maybe just
scraping the bottom of the top 20 here.
Robin:
Let me suggest there's also a correlation between ways that status is measured
in the sense that potential people are more rated by endorsements of other
people who have status. And the other people are more rated by concrete
achievements than they can point to. And there's also a conflict there, right?
I mean, there's just a difference in style. So the second group, people who
have status on the basis of their concrete achievements, even though they
weren't rated with potential, in part that was because they didn't have
contacts and endorsements. And their status isn't coming so much through the
contacts and endorsements. It's coming more from the concrete things they did.
Whereas the people with potential, the main reason initially they were rated
as having potential is often the contacts and endorsements that they had. And
then later on, they have a stake in continuing the contacts and endorsement
mechanism of choosing people. Whereas the other people more want to say, no,
you know, we're old enough that you should be looking at what we've actually
done. and that there's a conflict between those two groups in terms of how we
should be picking people.
Agnes:
There's a couple of distinctions that cut across each other, right? So I was
thinking, well, there's kind of humanities versus tech, and I see the anxiety
over the status that you have as more of a humanities thing, and the
resentment over the status you don't have as more of a tech thing. They're
both in both places, but just more so. But then there's also the, are you at
the beginning or at the end of your trajectory, right? So I gave like the
first year graduate student versus Elon Musk, so we got like the very far
extremes of the two possibilities. So those are both relevant, but they cut
across each other.
Robin:
So if we think of STEM, there's a lot of people in academia who rise through
the academic ranks and mainly initially have potential and endorsement. And
those aren't the people you're thinking of in tech. Tech people you see are
people who more took a big risk. And then the ones you hear about are the ones
whose startups succeeded. And so that's not, most people who have high STEM
status, are people who, you know, grew up through the standard hierarchies
like you would have philosophy and then have a tenured position at some
prestigious university or something. That's not the people you hear about in
tech though.
Agnes:
Right, okay, fair. And so I didn't say STEM, I said tech. And so maybe if it
was the STEM people in my university, maybe I would have something similar.
But I think there's more of the anxiety.
Robin:
We could look at other areas, like if you think about literature, say J.K.
Rowling, right? Apparently she was mostly a nobody until she managed to
successfully publish her first book in her 60s after rejecting by 20
publishers or something. And compare that to someone who's a literature
professor somewhere who then has all these endorsements and all these
connections. And then you're going to see a different attitude there too about
how you pick people and which things should count. And many literature
professors are quite sensitive about the fact that they've never published a
book that many people bought.
Agnes:
Right, so how do we distinguish? I mean, in some way, an achievement is an
endorsement, right? If I make a product and a lot of people buy it, that's an
endorsement, at least if there's not some big consumer recall or something,
turns out my product was terrible.
Robin:
Well, we're thinking about status endorsement. So status or only works high
status people can create your status by endorsing you status-wise. Just
ordinary people endorsing you, that's not the same kind of process.
Agnes:
I see, so there's maybe two different kinds of endorsement. There's the
populist endorsement of, you know, some, not massively hate Twitter, but
whoever makes Twitter, then the people, everyone flooding to it is an
endorsement of that product.
Robin:
Somebody applying to grad school in your department who showed you how many
Twitter followers they have, that's just not going to get you into their
department. But if their professor at their undergraduate school writes them a
letter of recommendation, that'll count for you, right? So clearly for that
kind of status potential, popularity doesn't count much.
Agnes:
Right, right. So the achievement is more like popularity and the status is
more like, the endorsement is more like popularity with the right group of
people.
Robin:
Right, or basically the claim that there's potential. So in some sense, that's
what a letter of recommendation is. It's a high status person saying you have
a potential to become high status.
Agnes:
Right, so it's not, the endorsements by high-status people aren't just
endorsements, they're not just this is good, this is potentially good.
Robin:
Right.
Agnes:
Yeah. That's true, that's true. Though, if you think of a tenure file, the
tenure file is not evaluated on potential, right? You're evaluating it on did
it actually achieve something, and that is being evaluated by the high-status
people in the field.
Robin:
Although even there, people don't tend to want to use downloads of the paper
or even number of citations in doing tenure file evaluations. They mainly want
high status people to sort of directly say, this looks good.
Agnes:
Right, but Mike, what the point is, what those people are saying or not, this
is potentially something else. They're saying this is good.
Robin:
Actually, no. I've heard a number of tenure cases, and often they are speaking
to potential. That is, in fact, a substantial part of, they say, this person
has done this good work, and we expect it to continue, and we think great
things about where this person will go.
Agnes:
fair. Yeah. So so I want to I want to speak to like a deeper thing that I
think is an issue in this kind of potential which is that I think that there's
a kind of energy and excitement of being around young people or being around
the future. There's a way in which the young people are the future, right? And
you're tied to the future and the good things that they're going to do remind
you that there are still good things that are going to happen. It's like this,
if you wake up in the morning and you're in a bad mood, and you're grumpy, and
you don't even want to get out of bed, and everything seems terrible, and you
think to yourself of some good thing that's going to happen that day or later
that week or even in a month or whatever, that might get you out of bed. You
might be like, yeah, but if you think of a good thing that happened like the
day before or the week before, that's not going to get you out of bed, right?
And so there's this feeling of like, Good things only matter if they're in the
future. And there's a big philosophical paradox about this. It's called the
thank goodness that's over problem. Why should the dental pain be worse if
it's in the future than if it's in the past? It is. There's just a way that we
operate with indexicals where, you know, the fact that it's here versus there
in the future, like, from a space-time worm point of view, they're not
different, but from a, you know, human being, like, going through that, they
are different. And so the fact that the good thing is in the future is just
makes it more attractive and appealing to us in a way.
Robin:
So you and I were talking to a historian recently, and I was saying how
interesting it is to study the future because you could do something about the
future. So I think part of the energy in things yet to come is the idea that
you could in fact influence them. And so your passion and energy is relevant,
but many scholars are put off by the future and they like the solidity of an
actual past they can grab hold of and it feels more real to them.
Agnes:
No, I think they really care and are invested in the future, but that takes
the form of these young people. That is, I think if you want to know what does
it mean for a person to care about the future, it is for them to care about
the next generation of whatever it is they do. That's the future made
concrete. So they have a couple different requirements. It needs to be future
oriented, but it also needs to be concrete enough that there are real people
involved and people don't feel like The people that are going to be born in
the future are real. Yeah, they will be, but they're not now. And so it's both
of those are- We agree there is this- I'll just add one thing, which is, this
just reminded me of when I was little and my mom, my grandparents lived in
Hungary and we would go visit them. And one time my mom did a surprise visit
where she didn't tell her mom that we were coming. And she's like, she wanted
to be surprised. And we showed up there and her mom was really happy. And
she's like, never do that again. And my mom was like, why not? Wasn't it? And
she's like, look, like a lot of the joy I get from your visits is anticipating
that they're coming. And you deprive me of like two months that I would have
been enjoying this future prospect where it's almost better for us to be about
to come than for us to be there. Okay.
Robin:
So, I mean, there's these two separate effects, I think. There's a youth
effect and there is a potential versus achievement effect. So, they were
careful to separate those in this paper we're looking at. And so, think of
somebody who is in a stable career and looks like they're just going to stay
there for a long time versus someone who seems to change careers often. Now
you might think we would see more potential and exciting possibilities in the
person who is changing careers, but in fact, they tend to be more pariahs. We
actually tend to distance ourselves more from the people who change even
fields of work or whole career areas. So that's a bit of a puzzle here. So
there are old people you see who are young in the sense that they're likely to
do a lot of very different things in the near future. And so there's potential
there, but often that's not so encouraging to the people around them. There's
a way in which people want stability from people, even if that's at the
expense of potential.
Agnes:
Yeah. Yeah, I mean, maybe they want, like, to have a grip on your trajectory.
So, you know, if you're just gonna, like, I don't know, you're gonna, like, be
in academia for a while, then you're gonna start a company, then you're gonna
go into government. You know, you're just doing all these different things. I
don't have a sense of, like, the value that you represent to me.
Robin:
So that seems to be the opposite of this breadth potential point you made
initially. We often like people with a narrow potential. They have a high
potential, but we still feel that we can predict where it'll go. And somebody
whose potential is less directed, less constrained, maybe we're less
interested in that kind of potential.
Agnes:
So I think there's different kinds of breadth. So I gave this example where
it's like you're in academia, and then you're running a company, and then
you're running for government. And whereas I think even within academia,
people who have certain kinds of breadth are celebrated, like intellectual
breadth where they can pull a bunch of things together, that is celebrated.
Robin:
But my point was that that breadth should then give you potential credit. You
should have the energy of potential with respect to them. But do we? I mean,
we can admire their breadth, but does that fact that they have that range of
possibilities make us have the excitement about the potential?
Agnes:
Maybe, I think if we're like, we know that they have a bunch of, we have to
kind of know that they are still planning to do new stuff. But people are,
like I just finished a book, and lots of people are asking me about my next
book. And they're kind of more excited about my next non-existent book. They
want to hear, what direction am I going? Right, they're interested in my
potential, not the actual book that's done. And so, but like, you know, maybe
I'm still whatever, I'm still young enough where they can still predict that I
would do something like that. But at a certain point, they'll just, I mean, I
think the age, I see that, you know, like use the potential are different, but
there's a correlation. And so people are using age to make guesses about
whether you have potential. And if there was an old person, but it was very,
very clear they were still very productive and they were still going to do
more stuff and they had breath, I think that they might get the potential bump
or whatever.
Robin:
So I still, the last theory I gave, I still think is relevant here in the idea
that a young person with potential is this package of a person with ability
and a future and a set of social supports around them who've endorsed them to
have this potential and therefore the plausible path ahead of them where they
will get good positions and good opportunities. and good partners, and that
that's an especially attractive package. It's less, it's not just about them
as a person and what they'll do, but that they are connected and tied in to a,
you know, a conveyor belt even that, uh,
Agnes:
I think that if you came across a person who was very promising, like
intellectually and sort of personally, they had a good ethos or whatever, but
they didn't have a lot of supports, that would in a way be even more exciting.
Robin:
Well, if you could bring them in and you do this for us.
Agnes:
We even have a word for this in grad admissions, a diamond in the rough, where
it's like if somebody went to a really low-ranked school, we're excited about
that. That's better. Because we can be the ones who discover them and maybe
the other schools didn't and so maybe we can get them. So I think that we
like, but that's just to say that the other explanation, then the other
explanation kicks in, namely we have more influence.
Robin:
At a cheaper price.
Agnes:
Right, exactly.
Robin:
So I think we could call this the potential of potential. that there's an
especially strong attraction, not just of someone with potential, but with the
potential of potential. Someone with potential is widely acknowledged to have
potential, but someone with the potential of potential might not be so widely
acknowledged, and then you could convert them to someone who has potential and
is widely acknowledged to have such, and you could be gaining on that
transition there.
Agnes:
Right, right, right, right. We like discovering people.
Robin:
That's more the stock price idea of you're trying to get things at a low price
before the price rises.
Agnes:
Yeah, right. Okay, good. But I guess I do just think that the thank goodness
that's over element is there, is at the kind of the deepest level, is that
somehow good things that are in the future are in many contexts more appealing
to us than good things that are in the present, even if the one in the present
is more of a sure deal and the one in the future is less so. It's an opposite
effect, because we also have the like, you know, people be thinking too short
term and taking the, you know, avoiding going to the dentist because it'll
hurt, but it'll hurt way more if you, right? So both effects exist. But we do
have this kind of estimation of, yeah.
Robin:
So I think this can help us understand fashion a little better. So the idea of
fashion is that there are current fashions and then there will be future
fashions, but we're not that sure what the future fashions will be. And then
there's these various candidate origins for the future fashions. And we can
sort of bet on one of the future fashions by becoming more associated with it.
And that's the way that even if you're older, you can sort of generate
potential. You're sort of generating uncertainty. Fashion generates
uncertainty. It means that there'll be this, you know, volatility of who is,
who ends up high and who ends up low. And in a world with a lot of fast
changing fashion, even older people can have a lot of potential to be the
origin of a new fashion. That is, if the thing they start ends up becoming
popular, now you can sort of get in on early. And even if they're 45, but
still by the time they're 60, their fashion will have peaked and you will have
gotten in early, but they're not young.
Agnes:
Right. And maybe if you're old, like, you know, you heard how I was
disparaging old people at the beginning of this conversation where I'm like,
ah, the old people in the audience, what do I care about them? I can't change
their minds. And so those old people know that, right, about how people are
going to think of them. And so they have to be thinking to themselves, well,
given that I can't influence other people by way of them directing their
speeches. I mean, how am I going to influence them? And then this might be a
very likely route, which is finding the young people and having them remember
that you were the one who discovered them or whatever.
Robin:
Right. So that's with respect to young people, but I was just pointing out
there's a separate effect of these old people. They could play the fashion
game and be the origin of a new fashion, and that means they can be the center
of potential.
Agnes:
But isn't being the origin of a new fashion the same thing as operating via
the young people?
Robin:
I mean, fashions don't have to be, you know, embodied in young people. I mean,
old people could just be fashions.
Agnes:
I see. I see. Right. So, for example, if you're, like, very excited about
shout GPT.
Robin:
Right.
Agnes:
Mostly excited about what it's going to do in the future, what the next
version is going to be.
Robin:
Right, so you kind of jumped on the AI bandwagon before other people did.
Agnes:
Right, right, right, right. I see a lot more people being like, look, it can
pass this test, it can pass this test, where those tests are things where the
person themselves didn't need help passing it, right? They didn't know how to
pass, so they weren't using it to pass the test. They just kind of knew about
other things that it can do. But as opposed to showing like, here's a good
thing that it did already where it was useful to me. It taught me something or
whatever. There's a lot more of the, it passes the test or the, what's the
next version gonna do? So that that is, that would be an old person's way of
staying new is jumping on that bandwagon early. So it's just that there are
other band magazines and Jumbom besides People. People is one of the band
magazines.
Robin:
Another example is movies. I think much of the marketing of movies is centered
around this, you can't be sure this won't be really popular. Don't you want to
have seen it before other people? Do you want to be listening to other people
tell you how it's such a great movie or do you want to be one of the first
people to have seen it so it's the potential of the movie? As opposed to like,
I like to go through historical records and find the most popular movies ever
and watch those, but hey, that doesn't give me much potential excitement.
Agnes:
Right. Except like, you know, I don't know.
Robin:
I could rediscover them and tell everybody, hey, this is a neglected gem.
Agnes:
Yeah, exactly. Exactly. Like you can just, you know, find what's underrated in
the past and then you can be the missionary of that. And I don't know, it
seems like people are, the business of rediscovering old movies is like a big
business. So that's a plausible way.
Robin:
I guess there's rediscovering old intellectuals and philosophy is one of the
things.
Agnes:
That's right, that's a huge thing. We're constantly discovering new early
modern women philosophers.
Robin:
And then you, there's the potential of being the person who first discovered
them or first called attention to them. Even if you're 50 years old, you could
still have that potential and people can be excited about your potential
there.
Agnes:
When I was in high school, I thought I discovered Immanuel Kant, because I
would talk about him to people, and people in high school hadn't heard of him.
And so I got to college, and people were like, what do you want to study? And
I was like, well, I want to be a physics major, but I also am going to study
this guy. His name was Immanuel Kant. He was a philosopher, and he was German,
and here are some of his ideas. And they were like, yeah, we've heard of Kant.
you know, kind of like witheringly, like, have you read the Critique of Pure
Reason, which I'd only read like the Groundwork to the Metaphysics of Morals.
It's like this little short book. And he wrote all these huge books that I
didn't even know he'd written. So but I was just really proud that I was going
to be the one to, you know, share Kant with the world. It was it was not to
be.
Robin:
So as a classicist to some degree, is this taste for potential something that
was just as strong in the ancient world?
Agnes:
They were super into like teenage boys. I mean, also as being sexy, but also
as like potential leaders and almost like sucking up to them because like
they're the future.
Robin:
Actual present leaders. Sorry, something just cut you off. Restate the thing
you just said. They were even more excited than interacting with their present
leaders.
Agnes:
Yes. To some degree, yes. But it was also occurring to me that even the book
that I just wrote, which is about Socrates, is a little bit me doing that. I
love being in that position. I'm like, I'm going to introduce you to a guy
called Socrates. I have to kind of do quite a bit of a song and dance to
justify that move, because people have heard of him, but I'm like, no, you
haven't really heard of him.
Robin:
A new angle on him or a new way to see him.
Agnes:
Exactly. I think I can introduce you to him. That's a thing that appeals to us
to do. So clearly to me, since I'm doing it.
Robin:
So I guess this is a reason why stuff changes. In the discussion of fashion,
there's this question of whether fashion is wasteful because some kinds of
changes are innovations that last and accumulate, but then other kinds of
changes just seem to go around in a cycle or something. And then critique
says, well, you can gain some reputation and fame for being the beginning of a
new fashion, but that's at the expense of somebody whose fame is declining
when yours rises. And there isn't an overall social benefit if you're just the
hemlines go up, the headlines go down, hair gets long, hair gets short. The
question is, what is the social value of fashion? And that's relevant here.
The question is, this extra excitement we have for the youth, to what extent
is that productive because we'll actually make a better generation of youth?
Or to what extent is it a bit of a waste because we have to keep pretending
things are different when we're just cycling in the same space?
Agnes:
I mean, you do want people to feel invested in educating the youth, right? And
to feel like enthusiasm to be talking to them. And so whoever the youth are, I
mean, every single time you want them to be invested in it. So maybe it's not
so inefficient as the Hemlines case.
Robin:
Well, here's a related issue. In a world where people just did things and were
rated on what they actually did, you might think that abilities were well
calibrated to tasks. You know, if people who write a famous, you know, a
really great novel are famous for the novel or people who prove a theorem are
famous for that, whatever it is, then, you know, we'll at least see a good
connection between the skills people are trying to acquire and the actual
skills that are needed. But if you create this whole world of potential, where
people are rated on doing things that other people see as having potential for
later being rated as someone who is rated high by such people, there's more of
this concern that just gets detached from the things that are actually useful.
And I think that is a criticism of academia and schools to some degree.
Agnes:
I just suddenly had a realization, which is that so much of your work is
really about your desire to bring about the world where people are rated on
what they actually do, like pay for results and- Yeah, yeah, exactly.
Robin:
This topic matters to me.
Agnes:
Right, like you don't like this whole potential thing.
Robin:
Well, when it gets, it has risks.
Agnes:
Wait, no, it has a downside. So I want to bring out the, that's why I think my
final line of explanation is important to keep in mind because I often feel
like in these discussions, the thing that you're missing from the story is a
certain, side or facet of motivation, like something like the difference
between wanting to jump out of bed and just kind of dragging yourself out of
bed. And there's just the kind of excitement and enthusiasm and energy that
people have around potential. Yes, youth, but I think because the youth codes
for potential and that that's connected to just the idea of a future of a
positive future where you should get that because you're a futurist that like
if we stopped caring about the future or I mean this hasn't always been true
that is there's many cultures where people just thought Things are not going
to be different in the future. Things are just going to go around like this.
This is how things are. It's how they've always been. It's how they're going
to keep going. In a lot of the ancient world, that's how they thought about
things. They didn't think there were going to be big technological
innovations. They didn't think human life was going to fundamentally change. I
have to admit, they didn't seem unmotivated. They did stuff, even though they
didn't see this open future. They didn't have futurists in this world. There
were no futurists. A futurist should, above all people, be able to appreciate
the special motivational oomph that you get when you think about the good as
coming in the future as opposed to happening now.
Robin:
But before we were making the distinction, say, between worlds where people
get rated highly early for their potential and then have a lot of social
supports and then move up through ranks, continuing with that social support,
even if they don't actually achieve their potential, and then other worlds,
like say tech, where it's more about just proving yourself by doing something,
I think those people are pretty motivated and they also are motivated by a
future. It's just a different path to the future. Each thing they're doing is,
could this be the thing that wins big and makes me famous and makes me a big
thing in the future? As opposed to the other world, people are saying, there's
these associates around me and I need to please them because if I please them,
then I will be rated highly in potential and And, you know, I need to do the
things they're doing and fit into what they're doing so that I will be rated
highly in potential. And that's the energy of the people who have high
potential ratings is they typically are focused on not the actual thing itself
that we might all value, but the proxies, the markers of grades or whatever,
you know, letters of recommendations, et cetera, that the world of potential
and give them. And honestly, I think that's less motivating. I am less
motivated by trying to climb a ladder and get letters of recommendations.
Agnes:
People are very motivated by that. I think the tech thing is usually about
proving to some VCs that you're some kind of material for something else. If I
was a person trying to do a startup or whatever, here's rationally what I
should think. I'm taking a risk, and of the people like me, you know, one in a
hundred or one in a thousand will make it big. It's mostly random. Or it's not
entirely random, but like within a certain group, it's random. And I may be
the one who draws the lucky straw or not. That way of thinking about things is
very demotivating. And I bet that that's not how people think about it.
They're like, no, I believe in myself. And I have this inner thing. I have
this potential. And there's some people in my world, maybe some VCs or
whatever, some people in my world, maybe some important people in my world,
who are like, yes, you have the thing in you. And the way that we get the one
out of a hundred to win is to make them think that's not what's happening,
that they spun the roulette wheel and won. And none of those people in tech
who succeed who are like, you know, spun the roulette wheel and won, they
don't see it that way. They're like, no, I had this inner drive or whatever.
And some people recognize it from the beginning. So I don't think there is
another narrative. Everyone has that narrative.
Robin:
I wasn't claiming that the difference was the narrative. I was claiming the
difference was what are the proximate signals that are being used to decide
who has potential and whether you have potential. In one world, the proximate
signals are people in an established hierarchy who then, you know, approve of
you and that you, you know, get an A in their class or get a letter of
commendation from them. And the other world is where you do concrete things
and then some of those impress people and some people hear about them and then
you rise in people's eyes by doing those particular things.
Agnes:
I think they're just kind of similar. They're different in the fine points,
but in the second world, you have to impress people. And in the first world,
you have to do stuff to impress people. And in both cases, there's people and
you're impressing them.
Robin:
But the stuff you do is different. That was my point. That is, in one world,
the stuff you do is closer to the stuff we all want done, that we care about.
In the other world, it's more a ingrown world where they just decide on their
own metrics and get to perpetuate it because they're the people who decide.
Agnes:
Right. That's, it's probably right that it's closer. The more it gets, the
more that other world gets kind of institutionalized, and they're like, you
know, tournaments of success with judges, which now all exists, and lots of
techniques growing for picking the next people or whatever, that world then
starts to imitate the academic world. So there's a kind of, Chinese can have
at the beginning that it's already doesn't have that much of.
Robin:
But there's still a difference. I mean, like one way we could distinguish is
say, you know, in China, for many centuries, they had the civil service exam
that people spent, you know, years preparing for about Chinese poetry and
stuff. And then if you won that, you could become an administrator. In many
other parts of the world, you become an administrator by starting out
administrating and working your way up, showing your ability to administrate.
Right. And so that's the difference between, you know, judging people on
something close to the thing that you care about versus judging them on this,
you know, other thing that at least, you know, you can make everybody have the
same standing with respect to, and you have a system about it and, you know,
everybody shows up on the day and they do the exam on the same day and you
control for things.
Agnes:
There is that weird world, but I don't think it's that so similar to academia.
That's its own weird thing. And, like, I guess if I had to say, like, in which
world is it academia or tech? Is there a kind of cult of potential and of the
newest thing and the newest young person who hasn't even been to school, but
maybe they, like, did something in their backyard or whatever, and then we can
dream about what they're going to do next. It's more tech than academia. That
has that. So I just think both these worlds have this. They both kind of
prefer potential over actual achievement. In tech, it may be a little bit more
connected to what's going to make me money and what reminds me of the young me
or something. And in academia, it's more like, what do me and my colleagues
agree on? I think they have their own forms of corruption, but they both
valorize and project fantasies of the youngest people.
Robin:
I've just been focused on the distinction between a world where the thing you
compete on is somewhat detached from the thing that the world, the wider world
actually cares about. versus things that are better grounded in that world. So
a world where you just start out writing and you rise by writing more things
that get published is a different world than a world where you rise through a
literature program and get professors of literature to approve of your essays
on literature, et cetera. That's just less, farther away from the world we
might care about, i.e., how well do you write? Similarly, you know, there's,
like I said, the civil service exam or something. So that's just the
distinction I care about, but it's not the only distinction here. And I agree
that in all these worlds, there's definitely the difference between
achievement and potential, and there's a valorization celebration of
potential. And, and, uh, that's just a fact about the world that we've been
talking about for an hour now. And I, yes, we don't have more to say.
Agnes:
Well, it's been an hour. We always have more to say.
Robin:
But should we stop? Okay. So we talk again.