Curiosity
Agnes:
Hi, Robin.
Robin:
Hi, Agnes.
Agnes:
So, when we finished our first season of this podcast, William, our technical
wizard, offered us the possibility of finding out data about our audience, you
know, which episodes are being downloaded, how popular they are, and you
wanted to know. You wanted this information, and I didn’t, so that we could
talk about curiosity. So why, why do you want to know, like how many people
are listening to us? Why do you want to know that?
Robin:
So I think my default is I want to know most everything, and that would be
adjusted by things that seem so unimportant. I wouldn’t want to fill my head
up with things that would be distracting from other things I care about, on
the one hand. So I don’t want to know about everything. So just like in a
trial, like you let both sides present evidence, but you say make it relevant
to the trial. And then, we have a limited number of rules of evidence, we say
you’re not allowed to bring that kind of evidence in. But the default is that
most everything should be brought up, at least if it’s relevant, unless that
violates one of the particular exceptions. And so I would think of that in my
life. In general, I want to know things unless they seem irrelevant to the
things I care about. But there’s a limited number of things I might not want
to know. So for example, if somebody is one to extort me or threatened me, and
they say, “If you don’t send me a million dollars in Bitcoin, then I will kill
your children.” I don’t want to know if they can do it. And I want them to
think, “I don’t know if they can do it”, because then I can just dismiss it as
hey, anybody could threaten me. I’m not going to give you a million dollars,
just because you can threaten me, you know. You’d have, I’d have to know that
you would actually go through with it, and then I might give you the million
dollars. But I don’t want you to know that I would know that you could
actually go through with it. That’s a case where I don’t want to know.
Agnes:
But I mean, in that case, wouldn’t… suppose you could know whether or not they
would go through with it independently of them knowing whether you know.
Robin:
In that case, I’d want to know.
Agnes:
OK. So we haven’t really come up with an example you don’t want to know. We’ve
come up with an example where you don’t want other people to know that you
know something.
Robin:
But often, those things come together, so.
Agnes:
Fair enough. But let’s just isolate the question of cases where you don’t want
to know. So I mean, you started with the example of the trial, and that’s a
very funny example to give of rampant curiosity. Because in a trial, they’re
actually very strict rules as to what can be said, what kind of information
can be brought in. It has to be relevant, right? And so, you know, this, with
respect to the example of our podcast, like the relevance was exactly what I
was questioning to which inquiry is the– is our facts about how many people
listen to our podcast relevant? What could we be in a trial? You’re inquiring
into a particular questions, did he commit the crime or something? What
question would you– could you be inquiring into such that that piece of
information would be relevant to that inquiry?
Robin:
So we will make a number of choices with a podcast, we’ll decide what topics
we do, and what, how often we do it, how long each one will be, and what style
to give. Should we disagree more, disagree less? Be more practical, be more
theoretical? Do we want to do this more or less? Do we want to quit? Seems
like for many of those, we kind of want to know what our readers think, our
listeners think. Or that is how… how interested are they, and which topics,
which kind of people are they?
Agnes:
OK, so let… I want to divide now. I think we have two points of disagreement.
The first is, should we care what they want? And my view is not really, not
too much. And then the second is, is it really true that the reason one wants
to know the answers to these questions is because one is gearing oneself for
those practical pursuits? And I think the answer to that is also no. That is I
think people just have curiosity about things like this. Like, you know,
suppose that you couldn’t look at how many Twitter followers you had for like
a year or something. And then at the end of the year, you could look at it,
right? And there was no decision you’re going to make on the basis of knowing
how many Twitter followers you have. But like, would you want to know, would
you choose to look? Probably you would, because you’d be curious. Right? So
we’re interested in that, in the kind of thing where people say, “I’m just
curious.”
Robin:
So what we’re talking about here is motives for getting information. And in
our book, Elephant in the Brain: Hidden Motives in Everyday Life, we do
discuss motives for news, and we do agree with you that people seemed very
interested in news they can’t use.
Agnes:
And facts, in general, that they can’t use, like this fact about our
listeners.
Robin:
Well, so people try to justify this open curiosity by saying, well, you never
can be very sure exactly what will be useful. So it’s often helpful to just
broadly collect things that you’re not sure which. So for example, you might
take a shop class. And in the shop class, you might learn a bunch of tools of
shop class. And we might say, “Well, why do I need to learn that particular
tool?” And so I might say, “Look, you’re going to be doing lots of things with
shops in the future. You don’t know which particular projects you work on,
just learn all the standard tools in the shop class.” And for collecting
tools, that’s a general heuristic. You don’t decide for each one exactly what
you’re going to use it for, you just collect a set of tools, and we do that
intellectually, too. So for news, the question is, overall, how useful will be
some category of things, not exactly which thing will be used for what?
Agnes:
I think it’s interesting, whether we can decide whether we model the
acquisition of information as the collecting of a tool, or the satisfaction of
a desire, right? And those are two different ways we might frame it. And
initially, you were framing it as more the satisfaction of a desire that is,
you say, I want to know stuff, right? That’s a desire that is satisfied when
you know the thing, right? Or you’re done when you know that, right? If you
want to know something, and then you know it…
Robin:
Right.
Agnes:
Right. Case closed. And your viewers… look, in general, I want to know stuff,
and so you have a kind of… it’s very much different from how you respond to “I
want to eat stuff,” right? It’s like, you know, when we want to eat, when we
want to buy stuff, but when generally doesn’t tend to indulge those desires in
a kind of unlimited way, we exercise restraint with respect to most of our
desires. And so you might think if curiosity is the name for the lack of
restraint with respect to the desire to know, then at least, prima facie, if
we were treating it like other desires, we would say curiosity is a bad thing,
which historically, and logically from where the word comes from, it describes
the vice and it was understood as a bad thing. But then if we have the second
model, where it’s not that you want to know, like, it’s not like you want to
learn how to use these various tools in shop class, it’s that you have various
goals. And those goals are sort of loosely related to certain means, and
you’re not sure in advance which means you’re going to need to satisfy your
ends, and it’s the goals that you want to achieve, right? And the knowledge is
potentially instrumentally useful with respect to that goal. So I think you’re
going to have to choose which way you want to go here. Do you want it to be a
desire or do you want it to be a tool?
Robin:
So for almost everything we talked about, we usually default to things being
instrumental. And then rarer things are sort of ultimate ends, means versus
ends. So in most practical discussions about starting the car, or route to get
somewhere, or how many things to bring to a party, or whatever, most of our
discussions are about means like… But sometimes we do have to talk about ends
because it doesn’t make any sense to only have means, there should also be
ends. And so a question in many areas is to what extent is something a means
or an ends. But in addition, we have this larger framing of evolutionary
psychology, which says that many of the things we treat mentally and
subjectively as ends are in fact means to other things. That is, we like sex,
but evolution made us like sex in order to produce– reproduction. And
similarly, we might say, evolution gives us curiosity, say, a general desire
to know things. And in our head at the moment, it’s “I want to know
something.” But there might be a reason why we want to know things. Evolution
created this general tendency to collect information relevant to important
topics. But we want to know, well, what different ends could there be that
evolution would have with respect to our collecting refresh info.
Agnes:
I like all of them.
Robin:
Exactly. And so that’s the topic of Elephant in the Brain. And so the usual
story about collecting information is exactly that it could be useful. And I
think it’s correct, as you’re pointing out, that if you look at how much
people are collecting here, it can’t all be useful. And in some sense, it’s
like a packrat, even like me in my office here. Some people just collect
stuff, and you ask them, “Why do you have that?” And they say, “Because it
sometimes might be useful.” And you go, “Come on, when was the last time you
looked at any of this stuff?” And then they have to admit, maybe it wasn’t
because it might be useful that they’re collecting, I think that’s exactly the
thing we should be asking about lots of kind of curiosity, what are the
different kinds of ends that evolution could have in mind for us even if in
our mind, it’s just curiosity?
Agnes:
Pause for a second. I mean you don’t have to pause that. It just pause, we can
just delete this part, but I just need my notebook. I can’t, like, I’m going
to try to function without a notebook and my brain just not…
Robin:
Should we have a pen, or you have a pen?
Agnes:
I think I have one. No, I actually need one. Thank you. Oh my God, this is the
worst pen I’ve ever picked a pen. OK, that’s a little better. Hold on. I need
to also put my phone on… so it stops doing this. OK, so I think that… your
thought is, we tell ourselves, we want the information for its own sake. In
fact, we are storing it away for some end, and we don’t know what that end is.
I mean, it’s interesting, because when you presented yourself, right, why do
you want to know how many people listen to the podcast? You gave both of these
answers, you first gave the “I just want to know.” And then later, you gave
the “It might be useful,” right? So let’s say just about this piece of
information, it, like… so he can decide whether it’s the packrat thing or the
good one is… do you think that you’re, you’ve evolved to want this information
for good ends or for bad ends?
Robin:
Oh, we just have to walk through it to figure out. I don’t know right off the
bat. So certainly, it’s true that people want to know what other people think
of them. That’s just a very common thing. So you could say, we’re talking and
other people out there listening, and I want to know what they think of us.
And that fits into a much larger framework of wanting to know how you are
perceived in the social world. And so that seems useful if it’s cheap, of
course. So that seems justified in the sense that people might be overly
focused on what other people think or hear about them. And they often
overestimate how much people care about them and are thinking about them. But
still, when people do think about you, it might be nice to know what they
think.
Agnes:
So, I mean, it’s interesting, because if we go back to the blackmail case,
right, that’s a kind of, there’s a kind of pathological interaction that
you’re worried about, and so you don’t want the information to flow back and
forth. And you might say, I’m sort of worried about the same… a pathological
that– interaction that can arise, if we learn like what topics people want to
hear us talk about, what style people want us to adopt, right? Then in effect,
we’ll become flatter or something, and it’s a kind of blackmail.
Robin:
Yes. OK, yes.
Agnes:
So like, yes, it can be nice to know what other people think about you, but
then that can also become a mood of shaping yourself into the sort of thing
that they want to receive.
Robin:
That’s absolutely correct. And there’s even a yet further level is that we
usually like to pretend we don’t care that much what other people think. So,
you know, whenever we, like, people often get negative news about what other
people think of them, or criticism, say, or people disconnecting from them,
they often say, “Oh, I didn’t care what they thought. I didn’t care about
them. I don’t need them. That’s not important.” And it’s a way of sort of, you
know, declaring that you can’t be so influenced by what other people think.
Agnes:
I wonder which of the two of us was more pretending that. And is it me by
saying, I don’t want to know, or is it you by saying you do want to know? It’s
not obvious to me.
Robin:
It’s not obvious at all. No. So a related thing I think is that in our larger
world, we have a substantial problem that people take listening to advice as
submission and accepting someone’s higher status or prestige. And as a result,
we are often reluctant to listen to advice. We go out of our way to act as if
we’re not hearing advice, and not to be seen as acting on it exactly. So for
example, a CEO or someone in charge, may be happy to take advice privately,
but advice given to them publicly, they will be wary of following because it
might seem like they are submitting to or accepting the leadership
predominance of this other person who publicly gave them advice. And so
because of that, we just may not be listening to advice as much. And this
could be a main explanation for the fact that we disagree. So I have a whole
research agenda on the rationality of disagreement. And basically, it looks
like people disagree more than they should. And one explanation for why people
seem to disagree more than they should is that they are reluctant to be seen
as listening to other people, and therefore, taking their advice, and
therefore, being submissive.
Agnes:
Right. I mean, sometimes, I’m really glad that kids don’t listen to their
parents’ advice. Because it’s like, if the parents had their way, they were
just sort of molded the children in sort of the reverse image of their own
fears or something, right?
Robin:
Right.
Agnes:
So like, and people really enjoy giving advice, I think. It makes them feel
like they know something to give advice.
Robin:
And my theory says it makes them look dominant.
Agnes:
Right. And so I mean, sort of, given all of that, like, how is it that… You’re
saying the way we know that people don’t take advice enough is just that
there’s this in effect, bad reason to avoid taking advice, namely, the
appearance of subordination. The problem is that given that there are bad
reasons to give advice, namely dominance, it may be that people are taking
advice at about the right rate, right? Because…
Robin:
Well, I would just say the larger thing we know is just people disagree too
much. And if they were listening to each other optimally, they wouldn’t
disagree, at least in the ways we see them disagree, and therefore, we have to
explain why they are not listening. And so that would be listening to good and
bad sources, et cetera, all this sort of information. But if other people’s
advice is bad, you wouldn’t listen to that particular advice. But that doesn’t
mean you actually still disagree.
Agnes:
So I think that we don’t really control our minds, but we have an illusion of
control in our minds. That is, we tend to think of a mind, we do, I think, to
a large extent, control at least, the gross movements of our bodies. So like,
I can stand up, I can go over, right? And I think that we infer from that,
something like, just as I control my mind, I control my body. Augustine has a
wonderful passage where he’s like, “My mind commands and the body immediately
obeys.” But the mind commands itself, even though the mind is the mind, and it
doesn’t obey. Right?
Robin:
Yup.
Agnes:
OK. So I think that… I’m just trying to get back to curiosity and to why we
might be wary of it, which is that like, our mind has desires to know things,
right? And we don’t control what desires pop up there to know. And we don’t
necessarily have faith that everything that it wants to know is a thing that
it would be good for us or that would be good to know. And if we just, if we
define for the moment curious– we haven’t defined it really. So you know, it’s
kind of, curiosity is kind of the indiscriminate desire to know, so to speak,
just because curiosity is often prefaced with just, right, “I’m just curious.”
The desire to know where in effect, if there isn’t any end to which this
information would be put, you don’t know what it is, right, that one ought to
be suspicious of that. Because the mind is… you know, you shouldn’t trust that
if it’s going in some direction, you pushed it or moved it in that direction.
Robin:
So I think the reasons to be suspicious of curiosity don’t have that much to
do with whether you control your curiosity, but have to do with pathologies of
gossip, and just excess news. Those are the concrete things I could point to.
So as we indicated before, it does look like most people just pay too much
attention to the news. And so, like my colleague, Bryan Caplan, pointed out
long ago, which I very much agree with him is that for our purposes of
learning about the world, you might as well just wait once a week, once a
month, once a year, learn about summaries, what did happen. There’s no
particular reason to know about it as it’s happening, because the summaries
you’ll get as it happened, just aren’t going to be as informed and thoughtful
as the summaries while after things happen. But we seem to be really anxious
to follow things as they happen in a way that isn’t very informative in the
long run. So we do seem to pay attention to kinds of news that aren’t the most
useful kinds of news that is just whatever everybody else is talking about, we
want to talk about, even if it’s not actually that interesting, and we don’t
wait long enough to summarize things. So it does look like we’re just paying
too much attention to the news with respect to the information purpose, at
least. So that’s a reason to be suspicious. And then I think the larger thing
that people are talking about for curiosity is just about gossip. The usual
context in which we would tell someone that you’re being overly curious is
when they are especially curious about somebody else’s business that they
supposedly have no business having business about. And that is the usual
context of which curiosity is criticized. And so we have a standard story
about gossip, how it can be problematic. And that’s the usual story in which
we say, you know, some things are your business and some things aren’t your
business. And why get so into somebody else’s business if it’s not your
business?
Agnes:
Yeah. So I am very skeptical of that story and of this fiction that there’s
your business and other people’s business. Like, that’s just I think, a made
up thing that people bring in when they don’t like people and certain things
about them. And so I don’t want my skepticism about curiosity to rest on this
idea that there’s stuff that’s someone’s business are not their business.
Robin:
So I haven’t given you the reason that is I haven’t explained the pathology.
Agnes:
OK, go.
Robin:
So that is the reason to be skeptical with gossip, it’s because of certain
gossip pathologies that we are aware of. And so the best way to make that
clear, I think, is the contrast between mobs judging people and the law
judging people. That is, I think, the main reason we introduced law was as a
corrective to problems with gossip. So I’d say the key thing that happens with
gossip is that, you know, somebody does or says something, and then rumors
about it spread. And then each person when they’re talking to the next person
in the network of spreading the rumors, they say something, and then they say
it with a stance about how you should have an attitude toward it, disapprove,
approve, et cetera. And your biggest incentive in that interaction, when
you’re listening is to agree with the person speaking. The person speaking
knows you, you care about them, you’re talking to them. This other person
you’re talking about is farther away, and your incentive is to just
immediately agree with their framing and perspective of this problem. And so
what happens is then some initial framing happens of this, and then that
spreads around. And people each have the incentive to sort of act
overconfident with respect to that in order to please the person they’re
talking to. And so the thing the law does to fix that is it has this idea that
there will be a judge or a jury, and they are not to come to a judgment until
they’ve heard all the evidence. And that is a big solution to this gossip
problem, and that usually, with gossip, where each supposed to come to a quick
judgment based on just knowing a little bit about the situation, and then that
spreads. And if everybody seems to agree, then we all do something. But with
law, the idea is there. There could be other things that each of us hasn’t
heard in the gossip network, and the single judge or jury will hear a lot
more, and they will wait to the side until after they’ve heard it. And so the
law is fixing this problem with gossip. But of course, another way to fix a
problem with gossip is just to tell people not to gossip, which, of course,
many societies have tried to disapprove of certain kinds of gossip, and you
know, not very successfully, but it’s a common norm.
Agnes:
So I want to distinguish a kind of practical role that, let’s say, let’s call
this like trivial or incidental information or hunger for trivial or
incidental information, a practical order that could play. For instance, as
you just described it could play a role in mobs punishing someone or
something, and then a kind of just wanting to know the thing, right? So let’s,
first, the practical role. So if we go to news, for instance, like, OK, some
people want to know the news in the “just wanting to know,” way, right, where
like, they just always want to know what’s happening to feel up to date or
something, right? But I think a lot of people, especially a lot of like public
intellectual type people, I think they always want to be making sure that
they’re fighting like next week’s war, right, like, not last week, and not
this week, but they’re like, they’re trying to push the edge of where we’re
going. So they want to be influential, right? And so the desire to know the
news is not so much a desire to have like an accurate understanding of where
things stand, which I think you’re right would maybe be best achieved with
periodic check ins, but a desire to be a kind of player, right? And so, you
know, that will be on the practical side, right. Now, similarly, on the, with
like the gossip, right? So there’s news and there’s gossip, and then there’s
the practical and the theoretical in both areas. And with the gossip, so I
think a great example of the kind of purely theoretical gossip in Plato’s
Republic in Book Four, he tells the story of Leontius, which is this guy, who
was riding by and he saw some corpses, and he was really tempted to look. He
wanted to look at the corpses, but he also thought it was shameful to look at
the corpses. And then finally, he turns and he yells at his eyes. And he says,
“Have your fill, you wretches!” Like, to his eyes, he’s like disgusted by
because they want to like soak up this information about the corpses, right?
So there’s a case where, what Leontius is saying is that I have this shameful
informational desire, right? I want to, I’m trying to take in information that
is bad for me to be taking in. Not because I’m going to punish anyone or do
anything with it, right? That’s just the theoretical side. OK. So we have news
and then we have sort of salacious information. And then in both cases, we
have both a kind of practical and a kind of theoretical prediction in relation
to it. And I guess the theoretical one would correspond to the thing that we
were talking about as being an end, right? Whereas, the practical will be the
information is being put to some use. Do you think that there’s any case to be
made, like, where do you think the real dangers lie in each of those cases?
Robin:
Well, first, I just want to mention the concept of rubbernecking, which is
exactly….
Agnes:
Yeah, it’s exactly rubbernecking. Leontius is the original rubbernecker.
Robin:
Right, so all the cars are stalled next to an accident, and everybody says if
only those other people up there would not rubberneck, and they would just go
through, then we could all go through faster. So that’s a case where we’re all
hurt by this curiosity. And we might say, what’s the point of this curiosity?
It’s a waste.
Agnes:
Right, exactly. I mean, that’s a perfect image of what is wrong with
curiosity.
Robin:
What can go wrong actually.
Agnes:
It’s rubberneck– right, what can go wrong is rubbernecking. It’s that we think
of… I mean, if you compare the hoarder in the office, like when you have too
much stuff in your office, and it’s taking up space, and you can’t even move
in your office, because you have all this stuff. To the person who’s
collecting all the facts, you might think, well, yeah, but it’s not like your…
it’s not reasonable to think that your brain, like that the capacity of your
brain is really the issue in terms of all these facts, and you’ll just forget
the old ones anyway, so you can just throw the new ones in. But the
rubbernecking case really gives you a kind of almost like a kind of visual
access to the idea of what it’s like when your attention is taken up by
something, right, that that’s not cost-free for your fortune to be taken up by
something. And so, curiosity is indulging in the ways in which the world takes
up your attention, right? And what we’re asking is like, is it OK to give that
free rein or ought one to try to, in some way, constrain it in the way that we
constrain our other desires? So.
Robin:
Being a public intellectual of sorts, I think this is a very important basic
question, which is, should I focus on what I think is fundamentally
interesting and where I have the biggest insights? Or, to what extent should I
adapt my conversation and attention to the topic of the week? So it’s…
Agnes:
Right. Right, right, also relevant to our podcast, right, do we want to
accommodate our audience.
Robin:
Exactly, exactly.
Agnes:
Yeah,. OK, go ahead.
Robin:
And so, there’s a saying in the think tank world that there’s no point in
issuing a report or a white paper on a topic that hasn’t been in the news in
the last two weeks, because just nobody will pay attention to it. So they have
to make sure their reports are related to the topic everyone’s talking about.
And you might think that’s a waste that is, you know, what, they have to
anticipate which things will be the in topic, when. And they have to sort of
be very flexible about what they think about in order to suddenly turn on a
dime to talk about whatever’s in the news lately. And that doesn’t seem
terribly useful in terms of using all their expertise, because it could take a
long time to think through a topic and come up with your recommendations, and
then your white paper on the topic. So from that point of view, it looks like
a bit of a waste that we are, like all supposed to talk about the same thing
at the same time. So we might want to say, are there contrary benefits maybe?
So you might think, well, if we could all focus on the same thing at the same
time, maybe we could settle it. For example, maybe if we all talk about
different things at the same time, then we would never really know if
everybody agreed enough to make a decision. So maybe there’s a benefit to
that. But for a lot of these things, it’s not clear that everybody really
needs to be involved. But it does look like as you indicated, there’s sort of
a status prestige thing going on here of, if you’re not talking about the
thing everybody else is talking about, you’re just not somebody. If you’re
talking about one of these side topics that people aren’t talking about, well,
you’re one of those you know, experts, assistant types who are good in the
background, but you’re not one of the big main people. Because the main people
are talking about the main thing.
Agnes:
OK, but I feel now that we’re drifting into a different topic that you, that
you gravitationally get pulled into a status, but I think it’s slightly
different from curiosity. Because, like this thing of like, are we all talking
about the same thing? Like, is there a kind of intellectual coordination on
topics, right? Like, you might think, well, there’s lots of different
occasions in which there’s a kind of coordination where everyone is doing the
same thing, like, Thanksgiving Day Parade, or Christmas, or whatever. Not
everyone, right? But like I’ve always enjoyed… I’m not a Christian, but I’ve
always enjoyed the fact that I like Christmas. There’s like Christmas music
and Christmas stuff on TV, and like, everyone’s doing the same thing. I think
that’s kind of nice. But it seems very different from curiosity, right, which
is the kind of the kind of letting go of the mind to be, you know, to be kind
of batted about by the forces of attention.
Robin:
OK. But this feature of the conversation having a topic and everybody changes
the topic together, I don’t think that’s so much about curiosity. I do think
that’s more about status. So think about clothing, fashion, or there’s some
other kind of fashion, right? Why do you want to follow the clothing fashion?
Well, the main thing is that the people who follow it first are seen as
especially knowledgeable and influential, and well connected because they
learn about the fashion first. And I recall anthropology of you know,
primitive “tribes,” they are often extremely interested in news. They, not
particularly news they can use, but you know, the top people in the tribe are
often putting a lot of time and energy into making sure that they learn about
things first. And so that they can go to other people and tell them about
them. And this is one of the ways they get status in this world is be the
people who give other people the news first. And so they might travel a long
way, or make sure people who are coming in come to see them first, it’s
important that they get the news first, and they tell other people first.
Agnes:
So maybe what’s happening there is that there is a kind of confluence of two
factors. Like, one of them is we know that people are curious, right? So we
know in terms of like if I want to get your attention, and I want to be sure
that you’re going to pay attention to me that you’re going to treat me as
somebody worth listening to, et cetera, I want to give you some things that
you’re going to want to have, right. And so, in effect, I’m a comment– if I’m
telling you news by making sure I get the news first and then give it to you,
I’m accommodating your curiosity, I’m kind of a flatterer almost, right? In
the ways that we would be doing if we would be looking to our audience to
figure out what they want us to say, we’re like, let’s say the things that
they want us to say, and then they’ll want to listen to us, right? And so this
kind of both the kind of status and the coordination of conversations comes as
a result of a collective accommodation to curiosity, right?
Robin:
I’m a little more suspicious of these collective stories. I’m more going to
look at the local incentives. But I definitely see a strong effect in small
local networks and say, in a firm or a university, that people who know about
things before other people are sure to go tell other people and they kind of
proudly say, “Look, I know about this before you do. I know about this before
other people. I’m somebody, I’m connected. I’m the sort of person you want to
talk to, and you want to have in your groups because look at the things I
know, before other people.”
Agnes:
Right. But, like, so I’m looking at a deer outside the window, which is why
I’m like not being totally paying attention to you. And the deer just
distracted me. I don’t know if you can see, it’s like there. Yeah.
Robin:
Yeah, I see it.
Agnes:
OK. Sorry, listeners at home, you can’t see the deer, but it’s there. And you
know, now, like my attention was just distracted by this deer. And I, so I was
paying less attention to you. And so, if we say, well, the person who has the
news and who’s saying, “I’m better than you because I have this first,” part
of what the reason they’re able to do that is because the other person is
distractible, i.e., cares about getting the news, right?
Robin:
Sure.
Agnes:
So, like if you knew it was Bryan, and you’re like, “Hey Bryan, I just find
out the newest thing happened.” Bryan be like, “I don’t care. I couldn’t care
less.” And you have now know less leverage or dominance over Bryan. Right? So
he has, in fact, made himself immune to this, through this stance in sort of
the way that I’m trying to make myself immune by not finding out the
information about who’s listening to podcast, right. So I do think that it’s
the kind of status system that establishes relations of dominance in this way
is piggybacking on curiosity.
Robin:
Of course, just the way many other status things do. So the person who looks
prettiest in the dress at the party is higher status because they look pretty,
right? So pretty, this is a nice thing.
Agnes:
Right, right. But, so this one I’m trying to say that like that, like, I think
you’re right, that we can get status relations that are built up out of a
response to curiosity. But our question isn’t about those, but it’s about
curiosity. And maybe your point is, well, yeah, but one of the reasons why
curiosity is bad is that it lends itself to these bad status hierarchies,
though you might ask, “Well, suppose we remove curiosity, would then something
else become the basis of it? And it might, right? The question is like, what
is the problem with curiosity itself? Let me frame it like this. If I look
online, OK, one of the words that I find most regularly associated with
something good and rarely used ironically or negatively, is curiosity. Right?
People adver– on Twitter, people advertise themselves as curious, people
always seeking to be in touch with curious people, right? So this is one of
the things you can say, it’s sort of like, there’s actually a bit of a
challenge in terms of presenting your persona quite generally, and that you
want to present your persona positively to people, but you also want to look
humble. And so what do you say about yourself if you’re like, I’m very smart,
and interesting? People are going to be wary of you because, oh, you think
you’re so great. But if you say I’m dumb and boring, then they’re also not
going to want, right? And so there’s this question of like, what hits the
sweet spot of positive things you can say about yourself that people will
still like you for having said them? And curiosity is like this perfect thing,
so you’ll find it all over the place, right?
Robin:
Right, I agree.
Agnes:
So there is almost a kind of, it has a kind of mythical positive status, where
we view it as like a fundamental good thing about a person. And I’m pretty
sure you’ve used it in this way to describe yourself, right, as one of your
main virtues is curiosity. So how do you account for it having this mythical
positive status if in fact, what it is the kind of indiscriminate giving of
attention to things that might not be worth it that allow us to be in effect
pulled into these bad power relations.
Robin:
I think you’re absolutely right that curiosity along with say, creativity, or
romance are just things that our society celebrates, that many ancient
societies did not.
Agnes:
Notice, you can’t use either of those other two to advertise yourself, though,
so.
Robin:
Right, because that, so in some sense, you’re claiming more ability perhaps
with the others. And curiosity, in some sense, you’re not claiming great
ability, although it’s perhaps implied. So I agree that we just have these
things that everybody is supposed to say is good. And that right, there is a
reason to be suspicious about them that maybe they can’t be quite as good as
they claim. And maybe we’re covering over many negative aspects of them. So we
have many sort of low motives for curiosity in addition to high motives for
curiosity, but curiosity is something we all are going to give default to the
high motive for even if it’s not.
Agnes:
But why? I mean, why are we so charitable in our interpretation? Like we
wouldn’t say the same about ambition, right? You give ambition, obviously, you
can advertise it by yourself so clearly, but curiosity is.
Robin:
I mean, one of the main distinguishing features of modern culture is our
celebration of innovation. Because innovation is such a distinguishing feature
of the modern era, compared to previous eras. Previous eras had innovation but
at a low enough rate that people didn’t so much notice it in their individual
lives. We noticed the world is changing a lot today, we celebrate innovation
as a cause of that, and we celebrate things that we see as connected
Innovation, such as creativity and curiosity.
Agnes:
I don’t think I could use innovative the same way in my Twitter bio. I could
call myself curious, people would like me. If I said I’m a big innovator, I’m
so innovative, that did not go over as well.
Robin:
And that’s interesting, because, in fact, we have celebrated sort of things
related to innovation, but not so much innovation directly itself. But if you
ask, if you look at the stories we tell about why curiosity and creativity are
good, they are because it’s innovative. But in some sense, innovative is less
of a feature of yourself to celebrate. So we all know innovation is kind of
random. We try lots of things and sometimes they’re successful. And so someone
who actually innovates is sometimes less impressive than somebody who has the
features that we tend to think on average to produce more innovation. So if we
think creativity produces innovation, or curiosity produces innovation, but
innovation itself is pretty random, then we might end up celebrating. Or
intelligence, for example, we think intelligence produces innovation as well.
And we’re better… and do you think those are impressive features of people?
People are more impressive and admirable when they are more intelligent, more
creative, more curious. And part of our era celebrating those things is
because we give them credit for innovation.
Agnes:
I think I’m allowed to say that… I’m not allowed to say that I’m intelligent
either or innovative, or ambitious, or that I have produced lots of great
innovations, or achievements. So I think there’s something distinctive about
curiosity, where, and it goes along with the way in which it’s often prefaced
with the word just, where the person who announces themselves as curious also
projects a certain kind of non-threateningness. And I think that’s really
important. So like, you don’t have to be… if I say to you, I’m curious, you
somehow don’t have to be afraid of me, you don’t have to be nervous around me,
and I’m not exactly sure why. But I think that’s really important, too, that I
think. Yeah.
Robin:
So this is related to a simple story of management, I would give, which is in
the modern era, managers present themselves as scientific decision makers.
They’ve got a spreadsheet and they’re filling in the cells, and doing the
calculation. And that’s what business schools set them up. So business schools
are full of ways for a business person to calculate better decisions for their
business. And so managers present themselves to the world and each other as
decision makers as calculators. And in some sense, that’s just really not
true. I mean, they do make decisions, they do calculate, but that’s not the
main thing they do. The main thing managers are politicians. They form
alliances, they have to create… you know, the impression that they are
powerful people and supported by powerful people. And one of the main things
that managers do is manage these coalitions. And so for example, when they
have a project, the scientific decision maker projection that they make says,
“Oh, I would be really interested in information that would tell you more
about which, whether this is a good idea for a project, and how I should
change it.” Their projection of this information collector decision maker
commits them to this image that “Oh, I want more information.” But if what
actually happens is projects happens because coalitions come together. And as
we can support this together, then they’re really not that interested in
information about whether the project is a good idea, because it takes a while
to put together its coalition, and they’re not willing to give it up quickly.
And so this is, in fact, what I see in the organizations world, because I’ve
gone into organizations trying to offer better information mechanisms, and
people think momentarily that that’s what they want, until they realize they
don’t. And so this is just a feature of a lot of organizations and things that
is, if we say, what am I doing? If I’m saying, “I’m a politician. I’m managing
my coalition.” That doesn’t sound so good, that’s sort of violating some
norms. One of the safest things you can say you’re doing anywhere is “I’m
collecting information.” “I’m analyzing information.” Who could be against
that?
Agnes:
Right. It’s so… see, to me, what’s so interesting about it is that I think
you’re saying, you’re in effect, saying, “ I’m not trying to dominate you.”
And you’re also saying, “I don’t know why I want to know this.” So you’re
presenting yourself as almost like a little bit helpless, like, I’m just drawn
in to know this thing. So there’s a kind of, let’s say that curiosity reflects
a weakness of our minds, right? We’re liable to be distracted. We are wanting
to know whatever is new for no good reason. And, but by presenting that with
just two other people, we actually make ourselves attractive to them, right?
And there are other weaknesses that are like that. I think you might think of
empathy of being very empathetic, and being very inclined to suffer when you
see other suffer as a kind of weakness. I think it’s not even seen that way
anymore, but it is. I think empathy is like curiosity and that it is extremely
acceptable to present yourself that way. It’s almost to the point of it’s now
being self-praised. And so it’s like this weakness that you want to actually
project to other people, because it’s actually a weakness, it’s a way of
selling yourself.
Robin:
So imagine in an organization, you know, there’s… Jones is asking about Smith,
right, to somebody, a colleague and saying, “Did Smith decide whether to do
this project or not?” Right? And in the politics world, it could be about,
well, it’s Smith’s is a rival, and I want to undercut him. I want to know
which angle to use depending on what choice they made here. But if I’m trying
to deny that, I might be like saying, I’m doing some analysis. But somebody
can come back and saying, “Why do you want to know that?” Right? So if I’m
presenting myself as a scientific decision maker, I’m vulnerable to people
checking that theory based on the details of what particular things I’m trying
to find out. It might not match these parts of the spreadsheet I’m supposed to
be calculating. And so then a defense is, “Oh, I’m just curious.” See,
curiosity becomes this defense that, like, makes people unable to see what
your actual reasons for one particular information were, because it’s just
this diffuse unspecific thing. And so it’s a best defense, when people are
trying to probe this excuse you’ve been giving is that you’re just there to
collect information. That’s all.
Agnes:
Right, good. So it is also a way to avoid revealing your motives for wanting
to know something.
Robin:
Particular things. Yes.
Agnes:
Good. But, so I think, like, just going back to curiosity more broadly, like,
if I try to think about, like, what is the thing that bugs me about curiosity?
I think of it as like an attitude towards knowledge where you have like an
itch, right? And you want to scratch that itch, and it’s like, oh. It’s like
that year that you spend, not knowing how many Twitter followers you have. You
have this itch and then you find out, and now you’ve scratched it. And, but
then what do you have once you scratch the itch? Well, nothing. I mean, it
just gone, right? And so there’s like a… you know, people talk about sort of,
kind of consumption in this way, too. Like, buying stuff can be a way of
relieving some kind of itch, you have to buy something. But it’s not that you
want the thing you’ll get, it’s that you want to relieve the itch of
stufflessness or something, right?
Robin:
So we could make analogies to say travel or consuming various kinds of media
and art. That is, if you say, “Why do you travel so much?” People will give
this sort of information rationale. “Oh, I’m going to see other cultures.”
“I’m going to learn how other people think.” “I’m going to see a wider world,”
right? It’s a classic rationale for travel. And if you say, “OK, now that you
saw those things, what are you going to do with that? What have you ever done
with all the previous trips you’ve taken?” And people can’t say very much. If
you say you went to the art museum, you saw sculptures, you saw paintings,
“Why did you do that?” “Well, I was curious. I was interested.” And now, we
say 20 years later, “So you’ve been going to the museums for 20 years. Now,
what do you have?” And you say, “Well, I’m cultured now. And then I say, “OK,
what do you get out of being cultured? What’s the point of that?” And people
don’t necessarily have much of an answer. They, many people are just
collecting all of these experiences and pieces of information and its
principle that they get. And they don’t have a particular reason and they
really can’t point to much, but they seem impressive in culture. We respect
those people. People are liked and desired more who travelled more, who have
seen more art, who have known more movies.
Agnes:
I’m also very skeptical of travel, so we could maybe do another episode on
travel. But I mean, I think one thing that’s interesting here is just the very
model of collecting, right? So it’s like, I’m not even sure you’ve collected
these things. And now that I think about it more, it’s like, at first, I’m
like, well, it’s better than the packrat. Because the packrat stuff takes up
space, whereas the curious person, the stuff will just go out, right? You
can’t overstuff your brain, but…
Robin:
But you have them.
Agnes:
Yeah, but then you actually haven’t collected anything at all, right? So it’s
like every day you check the news, and every day, you satisfy the itch to know
what happened, but it doesn’t pile up anywhere. It just goes, it’s just like a
river, you know. It’s like the Heraclitean thing, where you just step into the
news river every day, and it’s slightly different every day. And…
Robin:
The most striking fact is they don’t collect it into larger patterns, right?
Agnes:
Well, and people do. I mean, they do like take pictures. I think that’s a big
part of why people want to take pictures, because the picture sort of, in some
way, underwrites this illusion that you are collecting something in the travel
experience, right? Like, I have it, right? Even if you could get, and you want
it to be the case that you did the act of collecting it, right. So people
don’t want to buy the postcard of the painting, they want to take the photo
over the painting. Because then they have grabbed it in a sense, right? So
there’s this illusion that we can collect stuff, right? But in fact, we’re not
collecting anything at all, where I see trap. I mean, one way to think of
travel is a kind of escape, it’s a kind of escape, right? And maybe curiosity
is also an escape. There’s something that we were doing before… there’s
something that you and I were doing in conversation before I got distracted by
that deer, right, where, like, it’s sort of like a not doing, it’s an escape
from the constraints and the difficulty in the projects that you are engaged
in, so to speak, in your real life. And a bit of curiosity is like a little
bit of vacation.
Robin:
So I think one way to criticize collecting is to see if people ever use the
stuff they collect, right? Or even organize it. So, for example, you know, I
watch lots of movies. I collect movies over time. I want to make sure I’ve
seen all the best movies, right? But a test of whether I’m actually
accumulating thing might be to ask me like, OK, what patterns have you noticed
in movies, for example, right? Are the best movies romances or dramas? Are
foreign movies better than local movies? Do modern movies have shorter
sentences in dialogue? Right? And if you ask them, have you seen a lot of
movies and you ask questions like this, and they have no answers, they’ve
never even thought about those questions, you might think, well, like, that
would be the sort of thing you do with information. You’d organize it, and
collect it, and look for patterns in it. I mean, in terms of science, or
academics, right? When we collect a dataset, we don’t just collect the
datasets and make a bigger one. We try to look for patterns and structures in
it. The point of a dataset is to do things with it. So we might ask that about
the data people are collecting. We could say, “You’ve been watching the news
for 30 years. OK. So now, when there’s a big crisis news, how long do you
think it roughly last?” “I don’t know. I’ve never thought about that.” I mean,
like, if you’re not looking for patterns in all this stuff, what’s the point?
Agnes:
I think there’s a different point there could you could have. Because, so for
instance, I, there’s usually, like at any given time, there’s usually a novel
that I’m reading, say, all right? I’m, in general, not so into movies, but
usually novel that I’m reading. And, like, right now, I’m reading a novel
that’s about laziness. It’s about this guy who just kind of doesn’t want to do
anything. It’s called, the Oblomov. And I am, I don’t know that when I… what
will happen in 10 years. I don’t know how much of the information from this
novel will have filtered into some kind of pattern. So like, if you were to
test me in that way, I’m not confident you would say she has done information
collecting. However, if you were to talk to me now, today, you would find me
making reference to this novel a lot. It is, in effect, shaped like it’s like
a lens through which I’m viewing the world and different things are showing up
to me as salient. Oh, it’s kind of like when you first have a baby, you know,
suddenly you see babies everywhere, right?
Robin:
Right. Sure.
Agnes:
So suddenly, I see like the problem of laziness everywhere. And so it could
be, for instance, that we are walking through our lives, noticing, like,
noticing relatively little of the very significant stuff that we could be
noticing. And some of the work of these cultural artifacts is to attune us to
those things. So, but now I’m making a case for curiosity, kind of.
Robin:
So I once had a friend who is a singer, a good singer, and I asked why she
does what she does, and she gave me reasons. And, but then she was saying that
what she does should be subsidized, right? We should pay a lot more for
singing than we do. And the reason she gave was it’ll produce world peace. And
this is actually a common argument I’ve heard from an artist, which is just
that the more people are exposed to art, the more cooperative and peaceful
they become, and therefore the world gets better. And I just think of it as an
example of an overreach for an explanation. I mean, this seems pretty
motivated to want somebody to subsidize your career, and to claim it produced
world peace. I would be pretty skeptical of that. But I give the example to
highlight that whenever we’re explaining any behavior, we have sort of a range
of kind of explanations to offer. And we should consider sort of the typical
kinds of explanations, and their relation to each other. So I think the safest
explanation is to focus on an individual and ask, why might it be in their
individual interest to do that thing? Then I might want to think about, well,
why might it not help them right now, but maybe help them later, or help some
other people around them later? Maybe we can sort of imagine social processes
would encourage people to do things that help their associates, or help
themselves later. And then other explanations might be somehow it helps the
whole country or the world overall the history. And I’m just going to be more
skeptical about those latter explanations, because they require some
coordination to what happens. You know, it’s just more obvious that people
might do things that are directly in their personal interest right now,
because they’ll notice that, and then they get feedback. And the world just
fails to coordinate on doing lots of things that are perhaps good for the
whole world. And so, I and many other economists will first look for the local
explanations of what’s in a local interest and then ask ourselves, well, when
we put that behavior together, does it create coordination failures, a way
that we’re all worse off? And then, you know, are we somehow finding ways to
combat that? And do some of these behaviors help us combat that? So certainly,
we do coordinate on some things. So for example, for curiosity, we could say,
as I perhaps suggested earlier, the modern world gains from curiosity because
it promotes innovation. And therefore, the modern world has coordinated to
celebrate curiosity as a way to promote innovation. That’s a story you could
give. And that’s a world coordinating story about why we are promoting
curiosity. And according to my standards before, I’m going to be somewhat
skeptical of that, compared to what individual benefits people are getting out
of their curiosity. I can hide their motives about which kind of information
they’re collecting.
Agnes:
I mean, I’m skeptical of it, just because it actually doesn’t make sense to me
that curiosity would promote innovation. In order to innovate, you have to
really shut down your curiosity about most things in order to pursue
something, right?
Robin:
Focus on one thing.
Agnes:
So it’s like, yeah, at some early stage, you got to be curious, right? But
what’s interesting about curiosity is that we present it as being kind of
thoroughgoingly good as just like almost like a way of life, right? You
present yourself this way, I think on your on your web page, where you want…
you won’t be like, I’m a curious person. Like, I just, I keep at it. Like that
toddler approach of like, oh, new interesting things. I just keep going with
that, you know. I never kind of knuckle down into the innovation shut out
everything else mode. That’s something we’re proud of, right?
Robin:
Well, I mean, I’d actually say, curiosity is important for innovation, because
innovation has two parts, invention and diffusion. And in fact, diffusion is
the overwhelmingly important part.
Agnes:
And does that require curiosity?
Robin:
Yes. Because to diffuse, other people have to… you have to notice what is
happening elsewhere and how they’re doing things different. To diffuse
innovation, a practice has to move from one place to another, and that
requires the new place to be open and looking for new ways of doing things
that they might copy from elsewhere. So you…
Agnes:
All right, because they require curiosity, if you’re the innovator, or
something, that part of it doesn’t require a curiosity on your part. It just
requires that there be curiosity out there.
Robin:
Well, again, the inventor versus the diffuser. The diffuser, the guy who
adopts the new practice from somewhere else has to be curious about what’s
happening elsewhere. So a new kind of wheat, for example, spreads across
farms. And it requires each farmer to be looking around saying what kind of
wheat are other people using? Have you heard about a new kind of wheat? Oh,
those people over there using this new kind of wheat, it seems to be going
well for them? Maybe I should try it here.
Agnes:
So, I mean, if we think about like, what are the alternatives to a kind of
curiosity approach to knowing? Right, that is cases where we would, we would
not say, “I’m just curious,” to say. It’s not the case that I’m just curious.
I actually want to know or something, you know, whatever. It seems to me,
like, one kind of case, like in the case of, I don’t know, reading novels, or
watching movies, I mean, I don’t think it’s fundamentally the desire, it’s not
curiosity that you’re satisfying there. It’s enter– you’re being entertained,
right? And in fact, you’re not… I mean, you’re finding out what’s happening in
some fictional world. But like, the idea of curiosity is you’re finding out
what’s actually happening, right? So that’s, so entertainment, to me, seems
different from curiosity.
Robin:
But when we ask people to explain entertainment, and to say why are people
entertained by these things, curiosity is often one of the main explanations
given. So when we ask about fiction, what’s the point of fiction? People talk
about the sort of real structures of life and morality that are embodied in
fiction and that we learn from fiction, basically, at least what other people
are thinking about the relation of actions to consequences, and the moral
framing of them.
Agnes:
Oh, right, absolutely. So that’s sort of my point in saying that there are
different... We can say wanting to know, right? Because that always translate
into curiosity. Curiosity is a particular kind of wanting to know, the kind
where prefacing it with the word just would make sense. Right? And so if I
say, you know, if I want to know, like if my child is doing OK, I wouldn’t say
I’m just curious, right? If I want to know how to solve an important
geopolitical problem, I wouldn’t say I’m just curious. If I want to understand
humanity, and to learn like how others think and how to move through the world
in relation to them, I would not say, I’m just curious. I would say something
like, no, I actually want to know those things. It’s important to me to know
them, and I’m in some way invested in knowing them, right? So there’s a way in
which curiosity somehow the desire to know gets unweighted by further
practical projects or interests. And that’s how I was, that’s how I was
describing it in terms of like the itch, right? It’s like there’s this itch
and you scratch it, and then you’re done, right? Versus, no, like I want to
know because it has some kind of meaning or mattering to me such that, when I
do know, the knowing of it is the having of something that’s valuable. And
that’s different from a case of curiosity.
Robin:
So, with respect to advertising, we have the distinction between push and
pull. And with respect to say, search for customers, we have the push and
pull. So most people finding about our products is a mixture of firms pushing
the information out, and customers pulling the information to them. And most
cases are a mixture of these two. And when I was thinking about what you were
saying, I was trying to imagine where do I, would I put the word curiosity in
most of my behavior? And I actually think recently, it’s browsing YouTube.
Because that’s where I notice sort of my moving back and forth between having
an idea in my head, oh, I’m going to look for this. I’m going to look for like
all the different people ever sang this song and see how they compare, right?
And once I’ve formed that in my mind, I’m looking for that. And so it’s less a
curiosity. I mean, it’s personally a curiosity. But now, I chose, I pulled. I
said, this is the thing I want, right. But then there’s other modes where,
look, browsing the list of other things for this goal, I see another thing,
and I go, oh, that could be interesting. And I just click on that, because it
mentioned a thing that I… and sort of vaguely curious about, but I didn’t have
a specific goal for clicking on that. And so in that case, I’m being more
curious. And so the distinction seems to be that when I’m looking at things,
there’s a mixed initiative. Sometimes I have a very particular search. So
sometimes I search for articles, for example, on a topic. I have a blog post
and I’m going to write about that, and I think, oh, I’m going to write about
this. Let’s go look for articles about that. And then I typed in some words to
Google, and now, I get a list of articles that mentioned the topic, and I read
some of them. And so in that mind, I’m directing the search, I’m picking the
topic, and I’m looking at those things. Whereas other times, I just happen to
notice something and I go, oh, that could be interesting. And now, I call that
more curiosity. Why? Because it’s less at my initiative, and more in the
response to cues that come to me. But I am responding to cues, I’m not just
universally interested in everything. There is no generic curiosity, you know.
I respond to cues for particular… So the guy who walked past the dead bodies,
right? He’s not curious about everything, he’s curious about dead bodies. I
mean, that’s a cue that that stands out to him and draws his attention, right?
And so I think that, you know, are using the word curiosity is sort of just
hiding the fact that we have all these things that we pick up off and cues
that draw our attention, and maybe we don’t want to call attention to those
particular cues, and we just want to say that we’re curious.
Agnes:
Good, right. So I think that you’re right that the curiosity case is more like
the, you know, being pulled or something, or it’s… right, it’s not that your
initiative. And so the idea is there’s a set of things that you’re interested
in, where you didn’t decide that they were things that you should be
interested in, right? Like, Leontius, dead body case, right?
Robin:
Dead bodies?
Agnes:
But you still are, right? And so you’re passively subject and your attention
is passively subject to being drawn in right to collect information about
something, about a topic that you don’t think you you’re not going to write a
blog post about it, et cetera. Right?
Robin:
Right.
Agnes:
And you might think there are advantages to having that sort of openness, just
be part of your life and it would be bad to be utterly in curious, and to be
so well regimenting your mind that you’re incapable of being distracted ever.
But there’s downside.
Robin:
So let me throw in the word impulsive.
Agnes:
OK.
Robin:
Right. So in some sense, curiosity’s information impulsiveness.
Agnes:
Yes, absolutely.
Robin:
So think of yourself in the grocery store. You have the list of things you
plan to get and then they put these things at the front next to the cash
register that you might impulsively buy. Say, a candy bar. And you’re reacting
to the scene. So for example, Facebook seems to think that I will want to
click on ads with lots of skin showed by women. Because that little tiny
picture there often shows pictures like that. Right? And so, I could be
impulsive in that way, right? And so the dead body is an impulsive reaction,
right? So we might think of it as because it’s not your initiative, it’s not.
So one of the things you buy at the store that are not your initiative, well,
that’s the store’s initiative, that’s called impulse buying. And so, the rest
is impulse listening or impulse information.
Agnes:
Right. Good. I agree with that. I think curiosity is informational
impulsiveness. And we should start to see curiosity as being exactly as good
as we see other forms of impulsiveness, namely, I’m a whole somewhat
dangerous, though we wouldn’t want people never to have any impulses or
something like that. Instead of seeing it as just some kind of virtue, which
is how… we would never, other context, we wouldn’t call impulsiveness a
virtue, right?
Robin:
Well, so it’s our way of celebrating information by celebrating the
impulsiveness of it.
Agnes:
I mean, celebrating, we’re celebrating a particular way of responding to
information, right? That is, it’s like you could celebrate food by talking
over reading, or something.
Robin:
So we celebrate impulsive charity. So interestingly, I’m associated indirectly
with a large community called, Effective Altruism. And their criticism of
usual charity is that it’s too impulsive. And they’re right, in the sense that
most people don’t think very carefully about their charity or help. And they
do often respond directly to sort of visual clues in front of them, and people
like that and praise that. So that, you know, in fact, our theory, as we
described in the Elephant in the Brain is that if you’re one of my associates,
one of the things I hope to get from you is that someday, if I were in dire
straits, and you could see I were in need, you would help me. And so what I
want to see in you is that reaction of seeing someone directly around you in
need and helping them. So if I see that you go calculate the best thing to do
for the world and you help somebody in Africa, or something in 50 years, that
doesn’t reassure me much that you’ll ever helped me. And so your impulsiveness
about helping reassures me that if I were in need, you would help me. And
sometimes in romance, we celebrate impulsiveness. Somebody who’s very
calculating about who they want and how they help seems a little too
suspiciously calculating. And we often want somebody to be just reacting
directly emotionally to a romantic partner without too much calculation, and
we celebrate impulsiveness there, too. So there are these interesting contexts
where we celebrate impulsiveness, but not at the grocery store.
Agnes:
So I’m going to say one thing, and then we should stop. So yeah, I’ll give you
the last word after this. But one thing that I think is interesting is that if
you think of effective altruists as people who are criticizing impulsive
charity, I actually think there’s a correlation between that and the praising
of curiosity. That is, they’re the same people who praise impulsiveness when
it comes to information,
Robin:
Of course. Absolutely. I think we agree.
Agnes:
OK.
Robin:
Reader… listeners may not be satisfied with that, but we agree anyway, because
we’re not here to satisfy our listeners, are we?